Since she started studying African savanna elephants in 1975, biologist Joyce Poole noticed that sometimes an elephant would call out to their kin. Sometimes, a bunch would answer, and other times only one would respond.

“I suspected elephants had a way of directing their calls to specific individuals,” says Poole, scientific director and co-founder of the nonprofit ElephantVoices, but “had no idea how one would tease this out.”

Now, scientists have published new research in the journal Nature Ecology and Evolution that begins to do just that. The study provides evidence that elephants address each other with calls, or names, that are specific to the individual.

Names are a rare phenomenon in the animal kingdom, and the few species that use them, such as parrots and dolphins, mimic a sound first made by the recipient. Bottlenose dolphins, for example, broadcast their own signature whistle, which others repeat when addressing them.

Elephants seem to do something different: They use arbitrary vocal labels that have nothing to do with sounds or physical properties of the listener.

“We thought this was unique to people,” says study co-author George Wittemyer, a biology professor at Colorado State University and and chairperson of Save the Elephants’ scientific board.

FROM FRIDA TO ROTHKO

AUDIO BY MICHAEL PARDO


In human language, an arbitrary label would be calling a bovine a “cow,” since that word does not resemble, either physically or acoustically, the animal itself. A simpler label, which scientists call an iconic label, would refer to a bovine as a “moo,” since that’s based on and imitates sound the animal makes.

Arbitrary labels expand the scope for communication and provide a way of expressing abstract thought—and it’s likely true for elephants, too. “In terms of cognition, that opens all sorts of possibilities,” Wittmyer says.

What’s in a name?

The team drew on both recordings made from 2019 to 2022 in Kenya’s Samburu National Reserve and Buffalo Springs National Reserve, as well as archival recordings from Amboseli National Park in the 1980s, 1990s, and 2000s. Poole, a study co-author and National Geographic Explorer, made the Amboseli recordings.

In all, the team analyzed data from 101 African savanna elephants making 469 separate calls to 117 recipients.

The scientists focused on contact, greeting, and caregiving rumbles, which elephants use when initiating contact with an unseen family member, approaching another in touching distance, and nurturing a calf. These were the types of calls researchers thought were most likely to contain a name.

ELEPHANTS 101As the world’s largest land mammal, elephants have quite the commanding presence. But did you know elephants can’t jump? Or that baby elephants lose their first set of teeth and tusks, just like humans? Learn about both Asian and African elephants and the threats facing these hi…

Elephants produce a range of vocalizations, from trumpets and barks to many types of rumbles. Their structures are complicated. For example, rumbles are low-frequency sounds, partially outside the range of human hearing, that not only vary a lot but can travel through the ground and last anywhere from half a second to 12 seconds. That diversity can contain a lot of information, which makes them challenging to interpret.

“Our data suggest that, unlike human names, where the name itself accounts for most of the variation in the utterance, elephant names account for only a small portion of the variation in the call,” says study leader Mickey Pardo, a Colorado State University postdoctoral fellow.

“Elephants may cram more information into a single vocalization, so the name might be only one piece of a complex signal that simultaneously conveys other information.”

Though researchers don’t know how a name is encoded in a call, this study indicates it is there.

Plugging the data into a computer algorithm, researchers found that calls from the same caller to the same receiver were more similar than calls from the same caller to different receivers.

Hence, the acoustic structure of calls from a Samburu elephant named Frida to a matriarch, Donatella, were more alike than Frida’s calls to other elephants, like her cousin Rothko.

This “indicates that the calls were indeed specific to the individual targeted receiver,” Pardo says.

More data needed

The researchers’ new analysis found contradictory answers as to whether different elephants use the same name to address the same receiver.

One of several possible explanations is that different elephants use name derivatives to address the same receiver—somewhat akin to nicknames. (Read: “What are elephants really saying? First-ever library reveals communication mysteries.”)

“Just as the human nicknames Liz, Elizabeth, and Ellie have certain features in common despite being distinct words, it may be that different elephants address the same individual with somewhat similar yet distinct labels,” Pardo says.

Lack of data is an issue, says Tel-Aviv University zoology professor Yossi Yovel, who was not involved in the study. While he finds the topic “super interesting” and appreciates the attempt to tease out individually specific calls, he notes that “there are fewer than 500 calls, which is very little for such a study.”

“For many pairs they have only one caller and one receiver, so it is impossible to learn the ‘name’ of the receiver unless it is consistent across callers,” he says. “But they say it is not.”

“For a name to be a name, more than one caller should use it,” he says, noting that what seem like names can have other explanations.

For instance, if someone said “hi hi hi” to one person and “hey hey hey” to another, the algorithm would probably suggest one name is hi and another is hey, Yovel says. “But this is not the case.”

Name recognition

The researchers also conducted field experiments, playing back multiple recordings to the study elephants, with some originally addressed to them and others from the same caller that were not. (See “What elephant calls mean—a user’s guide.”)

The findings revealed dramatic reactions, showing that elephants do recognize their own “name,” and respond to it, Pardo says.

For instance, when Donatella listened to playback of a call originally addressed to her, she “called eight times, approached the speaker, and searched behind it.”

But when hearing a recording from Frida to her cousin Rothko, Donatella barely reacted, calling just once and not approaching the speaker.

‘A door into how their mind works’

“We’re really in early stages to try and decipher what’s going on,” Wittemyer says.

For instance, Poole wonders if elephants also have names for places.

“When they’re negotiating a plan of action, you can see them agree or disagree. But what are they saying? Are they saying, ‘I’m not going to go with you, I want to go to this other place?’” she says.

“This study opens a door into how their mind works,” Wittemyer adds. “We’re making a step to understanding them better, and maybe that will help us live with them better.”